J Eng Teach Movie Media > Volume 23(4); 2022 > Article
Lee: Using Films in Second-Language Learning: Perspectives Through the Lens of Transmedia and Cultural Learning*

Abstract

This study had two objectives: (1) to observe how the participants used a TV series to learn English and (2) to investigate to what extent they changed the way they used the TV series to learn English based on three activities or guidelines that a teacher gave for better learning. They were, first, that the centrality of the meaning (of the lexicon and of the pattern) is core in using TV series for L2 learning; second, how to learn conventional expressions; and third, that in using a language, language knowledge and world knowledge should be combined. For the analysis, the participants’ reports were used. For the first guideline, it was found that the participants primarily paid attention to understanding the utterances in the script. They performed poorly. In the second activity, the participants performed relatively well. In the third activity, when the participants were assisted, they were able to focus on patterns. After the activities, they realized that they could learn the importance of context and culture as well as conventional expressions. A pedagogical implication of these findings is that although TV series are expected to make learning enjoyable, language matters should take precedence.

I. INTRODUCTION

Thanks to media development in this century, opportunities to learn a second language are widespread. Particularly, the use of transmedia is gaining greater attention in general education, including in second-language (L2) learning. Before the development of media, people who wanted to learn a target second language (in this study, English) in a natural way had to visit native English-speaking countries such as the US, England, Canada, and Australia. They had to stay long in the country to learn its native language. However, visiting different countries caused economic and educational problems. This approach was a big burden, and consequently, many L2 learners could not pursue it and also caused college students to stop their college education. Fortunately, enough media have appeared as troubleshooters. They can function as one stop service platforms. Now, L2 students can stay in one place and encounter English naturally through media.
However, opportunities alone do not guarantee the success of L2 learning. L2 learners must also know how to use media effectively. For example, many studies have recommended films as among the best ways to learn a second language (Donaghy, 2014; Koyfman, 2021; Rosson, 2022). In addition, films also have the benefit of helping students to learn culture through multiple media platforms (Fleming, 2013; Jenkins, 2006; Lee, 2022). However, some English as a Foreign Language (EFL) instructors believes that films have benefits to learn language based on their experience.1 According to the instructors, many students inn film-based EFL classrooms, has shown signs of being too relaxed and laid back attitude. Nevertheless, some studies have found that films are good L2 teaching materials, but some experienced EFL instructors have carefully mentioned that they need guidelines with the consideration of linguistic theories to successfully use films to teach in the classroom.
Therefore, in this paper, some guidelines for using films for L2 learning are suggested. First, the ways in which students use TV series for L2 learning are observed. Then, whether they will change their ways of using TV series for L2 learning after they are given the guidelines will be investigated. Through this process, the pros and cons of using films, particularly TV series, for L2 learning will be discussed from the students’ perspective.
The linguistic theory that is employed is based on the rationale of usage-based models (Bybee, 2010; Langacker, 1988). Specifically, construction grammar, which is one of the usage-based approaches, will be the background theory. In this theory, usage is emphasized which means that, actual usage is important (Ibbotson, 2013). Ellis and Wulff (2015) used “the linguistic input [that] L2 learners receive” instead of actual usage. In this regard, films can be very appropriate materials for L2 learning because they are full of actual utterances.
Recently, construction grammar has attracted attention in L2 acquisition and teaching. In this field, construction is a fundamental concept for generally developing a second language. Ellis (2013, p. 270) defined construction as “form-meaning mappings, conventionalized in the speech community, and entrenched as language knowledge in the learner’s mind.” In this paper, three slightly different terms for construction learning are introduced: centrality of meaning, conventionalization, and encyclopedic knowledge. These comprise the guidelines for L2 teaching. If these three guidelines are accomplished in film materials, films will be strongly recommended for L2 learning.

II. Literature Review

1. The Centrality of Meaning

The following passage expounds on the current concern about L2 teaching.
If generative linguistics views syntax as being central to language, Cognitive Linguistics accords this honor to meaning. The latter seems far more natural from the perspective of language users. When ordinary people speak and listen, it is not for the sheer pleasure of manipulating syntactic form-their concern is with the meanings expressed. This does not, of course imply that grammar is unimportant in language or in language teaching. It is, however, helpful to realize that grammar subserves meaning rather than being an end in itself. (Langacker, 2008, p. 67)
Langacker emphasized that meaning is more important than form and that this seems natural from the perspective of students. Nobody would deny that L2 students tend to enjoy understanding the meaning rather than the syntactic structures of utterances. Langacker (2008) stated that grammar has its own meaning. L2 students will wonder how basic grammatical concepts such as nouns, verbs, and subjects can be semantically explained. Langacker said nouns refer to things, verbs refer to processes, and so on. Likewise, cognitive semantics does not have a modular view of language. In other words, it believes that all structures in language are basically meaningful (Lemmens, 2016). Wulff and Nick (2018) also argued that grammar has meaning. For example, they said that the simple morpheme aholic means “being addicted to something,” and the syntactic structure subject-verb-object-object means “something is being transferred” (p. 38). Thus, grammatical meanings sound somewhat strange, but they differ from lexical meanings only in degrees.
This argument benefits L2 learners in several ways. First, the distinction between grammar and lexicon has become less justifiable. Since L2 learners have difficulty in studying grammar, this message helps to reduce their burden of grammar learning. Lewis (1993) emphasized that “Language consists of grammaticalized lexis, not lexicalized grammar” (p. vi). In other words, the lexis plays a primary role in making meaning (Realm, 2009).
Cognitive linguistics argues that language should be explained in only one way. Lemmens (2016) stated that “All structures in language, ranging from morphemes to words to syntactic patterns, are considered […] inherently meaningful” (p. 90). Given that idiomatic and idiosyncratic expressions are neglected in generative grammar (Taylor, 2012), cognitive linguistics can be more reasonable in answering language phenomena.

2. Conventional Expressions in a Speech Community

The term convention means habit. A linguistic convention refers to a collective habit of a speech community. In other words, members of a speech community expect other members to use a language in the same way that they do, such as when they greet each other in the morning with good morning. Technically speaking, they tend to “conform to regularities in their use of utterance types” (Schmid, 2020, p. 87). Kádár and House (2021) defined convention as “shared understandings of meanings and practices” (p. 162). Naturally, members in the same speech community are supposed to encounter conventions every single day. When L2 learners go abroad to learn a second language, they learn these conventional things.
The EFL classroom is concerned with how to teach conventional expressions. Bardovi-Harlig and Vellenga (2012) stated that “Conventional expressions consist of strings such as no problem, nice to meet you, and that’s great, which native speakers use predictably in certain contexts” (p. 77). They said that even advanced students have difficulty in learning conventional expressions. Their experiment showed that instruction in acquiring conventional expressions is helpful because conventional expressions require pragmatic knowledge, which makes learning without instruction very difficult.
Dynel (2017) said that in textbooks for teaching conventional expressions in the classroom, fictional discourse can be used without a problem.
Essentially, fictional interactions on the screen are the fruit of the labour of scriptwriters, that is, language users who conceive characters’ interactions based on the workings of real-life communication and its psychological underpinnings. For their part, by defaulting making use of the same linguistic norms and assumptions as scriptwriters do, viewers (and academics) can make sense of the characters’ interactions, underlying goals and intentions, thanks to all the evidence purposefully made available to them. Production crews operate on the assumption that characters’ interactions should be tacitly accepted by viewers as natural relative to the socio-cultural context and should not strike them as being artificial, even if statistically infrequent. (pp. 55-56)
Dynel (2017) clearly stated that film-based materials can be authentic to researchers and L2 students. Even he used a medical drama, House MD (Shore, 2004-2012), in his experiment. He implied that characters’ interactions are more powerful than those of ordinary people. Because conventional utterances have high communicative value, interactions may come from authentic materials such as films.
Finally, conventionalization can be defined as a process that impacts the degree to which people in the same community conform to regularities while they are using utterances in their conversation (Schmid, 2020). If the degree is high, conventionalization becomes convention.

3. Language Knowledge and World Knowledge

When we humans speak and listen, we need world knowledge as well as linguistic knowledge (Willits, Amato, & MacDonald, 2015). In other words, the integration of linguistic knowledge and world knowledge is necessary to achieve successful communication. Louwerse’s (2008) experiment showed that listeners respond more quickly to word pairs (attic-basement) than to reverse pairs (basement-attic). From the perspective of linguistics, attic-basement or basement-attic are the same descriptions of a building in a vertical spatial arrangement. However, people in the real world prefer attic-basement pairs to reverse pairs just because they are more comfortable with attic-basement pairs. Beare (2019) said that words such as Adam and Eve, bacon and eggs, cause and effect, crime and punishment, dead or alive, fish and chips, and in and out, which have nonreversible order, are like collocations because they always go together. Beare stated that the reason for their being in that order is not clearly linguistic. The answer could be found in world knowledge. Technically speaking, interpersonal pragmatics deals with the importance of world knowledge. It asks why the same cognitive behavior is habitually and systematically presented in different linguistic ways (Locher & Graham, 2021). It is the responsibility of language users to choose appropriate linguistic forms that refer to conventional expressions.
In an EFL film-based classroom, interpersonal pragmatics should be considered truthfully. As is known, films reflect the virtual world, where a lot of interactions occur. First, L2 learners must perform L1 translation in order to understand the pragmatic meaning of words. Despite the blurring of the distinction between language knowledge and world knowledge, many L2 learners are not aware of the fact. When L2 students translate a certain film into L1, they tend to ignore L1 translation. If this happens, they may lose the communicative value of interactional utterances of characters. Therefore, teachers should guide their students to translate with caution.
Finally, the context of world knowledge can be generally defined as “the actual situational context of the linguistic sign(s) or utterance” (Kecskes, 2021, p. 605). The definition reflects that only linguistic meaning cannot be interpreted. Goodwin and Duranti (1992) stated that context is “a frame that surrounds the event and provides resources for its appropriate interpretation” (p. 3). It also implies that context is a frame for interpreting linguistic meaning. Gawron (2019) explained frames in greater detail as follows: “Frames are conceptual structures that provide context for elements of interpretation; their primary role in an account of text understanding is to explain how our text interpretations can leap far beyond what the text literally says” (p. 57). Taken together, L2 students should be aware of the integration of linguistic forms and relevant world knowledge. Considering that films portray a virtual world, they should not lose the world referents.

III. DESIGN

1. Background

Long-time language teachers have found that when they used films in the classroom, their students were reluctant to actively participate in activities (Lee, 2020). Film-based materials are expected to make students learn a language with fun, but the reverse often happens. Students seem to be divided between those who merely enjoy watching films and those who watch films to learn a language. Few students are inclined towards the latter. Nevertheless, it has been found that films are the best materials for learning a second language.
In this section, we investigate what kind of expressions L2 learners prefer while they are using TV series materials. They often tend to be disappointed with the results of their work as memorization is fruitless for them. To help the participants overcome such problems, some points were emphasized to them. As mentioned in the literature review, they were instructed on (1) the centrality of meaning, particularly of grammatical meaning; (2) conventional utterances; and (3) world knowledge.
Also, theoretical positions will be represented. They will take the rationale of a usage-based approach in which usage events or actual utterances are keys to improving L2 proficiency. Construction grammar will be particularly considered. As is known, students in film-based classrooms prefer idiomatic expressions. Since it is believed that linguistic structure consists of constructions, including idiomatic expressions, construction grammar belongs to the usage-based approach (Diessel, 2015; Hilpert, 2020). The last position is the meaningful learning theory of Ausubel (1963). When conventional utterances in film scripts are memorized, there should be a meaningful strategy for keeping them in one’s long-term memory.

2. Participants and Material

This study has three participants, and they are college students. According to their TOEIC test results they were all intermediate level. All of them have some experience using films (movies and TV series). In this study, they were asked to watch the American TV series Good Witch, season 1, episode 1 (Pryce, 2015), episode 2 (Lynn, 2015a), and episode 3 (Lynn, 2015b) over more than three months.
Good Witch was chosen because of its story (contextual meaning) and its characters’ utterances (linguistically and ethically good forms). Its main character is Cassie, who has magical powers. However, she does not scare her neighbors away. She regards magic as a gift, not power. She always uses it to help people. Her magic makes her neighbors feel comfortable. She is always thoughtful and sweet, as reflected also in her utterances.
From the perspective of L2 learning, Cassie’s and the other characters’ utterances may give prominence to L2 learners in that her message reflects a sort of touch to L2 learners. Prominence or salience is known to be related to successful L2 learning. This is why Good Witch is considered an appropriate material for L2 learning.

3. Procedure

The researcher instructed the participants to read the scripts and then submit expressions (words, phrases, and clauses) that they wanted to learn. He also told them to indicate why they preferred such expressions. In the first activity, after the researcher analyzed the participants’ submissions, he instructed them to study the scripts with a focus on studying the scripts. Vujic (2016) said, “In construction grammar, linguistic expressions are not necessarily syntactic forms” (p. 11). Simply put, every expression can be a construction. A word, complex words, phrases, and clauses are all constructions. Lexical items are constructions, and they are represented partially or fully according to their degree of specificity or schematicity (see Table 1; Fried, 2015, p. 978; cited in Vujic, 2016, p. 11). Syntactic patterns are described in the same way as lexical items.
Table 1 allows for better understanding of schematicity and specificity. Schematicity refers to symbols without concrete words. The second-to-the-last examples in the right column of the table show no word except symbols such as NP and VP. Specificity is the reverse. It consists of content words. In the first examples in the right column of the table, only words are listed. Each construction is represented by degrees of schematicity and specificity. The participants were asked to indicate the differences between the constructions according to their degrees of schematicity and specificity. They were not asked to distinguish lexical items and grammatical patterns. Because meaning is central, it can be a basic unit for describing each construction.
In the second activity, the students were tasked to learn conventional expressions in the script of Good Witch. This is one of the main concerns in this section. The participants had to distinguish between conventional expressions and non-conventional expressions. Conventional expressions are a subset of multiword units. However, the participants did not need knowledge about multiword units. Instead, they had to perform first-language (L1) translation. When they could translate an utterance as one word, it was a conventional expression. For example, good morning can be translated as one word in L1, so it is a conventional expression.
The important thing that the students had to keep focusing on was the differences between linguistic translation and pragmatic translation. Many Korean college students do not know how to distinguish between these two types of translation. Linguistic translation uses the dictionary meaning of an expression, whereas pragmatic translation uses its contextual meaning. More natural translation requires awareness of an expression’s pragmatic translation.
The third activity was about world knowledge. As mentioned earlier, language use needs to combine language knowledge and world knowledge. For better understanding, one example dialogue2 is presented below.
Ann: How was my speech?
Sarah: You were brilliant.
Anne: Did I lisp?
Sarah: You didn’t lisp.

(Conversation 1, The Favourite, Lanthimos, 2018)

In translating the above dialogue, some background about the two characters is needed. The first background is who Ann and Sarah were. The second is why they mentioned the word lisp. It is common knowledge that L2 learning is not possible with only a dictionary. Therefore, in the third activity, the participants were expected to be context-sensitive. Their reports had to include contextual information.

IV. RESULTS AND ANALYSIS

In this section, each participant’s work is analyzed according to the three activities.

1. First Activity: Centrality of Meaning

In the first activity, the participants were supposed to add the degree of schematicity or specificity. Of course, they did not have to use such terminologies. Simply, they had to write the reason why they picked a certain expression, and then the teacher analyzed their reasons. Table 2 shows the results of the first activity.
The participants did not show an ability to analyze schematicity and specificity. They misunderstood meaning as the meaning of the content. Even if they understood the expressions correctly, they probably could not answer them. Despite their mistakes, their data deserve some comment. Participant A intended to learn the meaning of take. He gave one example of this. Since he gave only one example, he did not generalize the meaning of take. Therefore, his expression was forced into the specificity domain. Moreover, he focused on only one word: take. This is not a conventional expression, as conventional expressions should be at least strings of words (Bardovi-Harlig, 2009, 2012, 2014; Edmonds, 2014). Participant B intended to show why he picked It’s no secret. He stated the reason in L1. However, he failed to show whether his chosen expression was generalizable partly or fully (a schematic matter) or specifiable (a specific matter). However, his selection was good in one sense: he selected the conventional expression It’s no secret. It consists of four words but functionally behaves like one word. Participant C’s selection also deserves comment. She tried to generalize left out with examples such as the following.
  • 1) I left out the carrots because I don’t like them.

  • 2) Have we left out anyone?

  • 3) I have left out the decimal point in this problem.

Certainly, she showed similar situations or uses of the phrasal verb leave out. She tried to generalize the phrasal verb in several situations. This could be in the schematic domain.
In sum, the three participants were not successful in improving the degrees of schematicity and specificity of their chosen expressions. They did not yet understand that language should be interpreted in one way (i.e., with one meaning). However, had they been trained as English major students, they would have had a deeper insight into language.

2. Second Activity: Learning Conventional Expressions

This activity was the most important for the participants because they wanted to learn as many expressions as possible. Such expressions were primarily idiomatic, and the participants often failed to form sentences with their grammatical knowledge. The results of this activity are shown in Table 3.
In this activity, the participants performed well. Participant A deleted the linguistic meaning of I guess. In the Korean usage of I guess, people do not explicitly express the linguistic meaning. They prefer implicit translations. Participant A followed the Korean usage successfully. It sounded natural. The phrasal verb bring up has several linguistic meanings, but Participant A took up a new meaning, upset, having considered the context. In a sense, he conquered his language. He created a new meaning. This is the right way to improve language proficiency. When an adult asks a child who does not know letters What is this?, pointing to a book, the child will probably simply say Book, not the title of the book. In his view, the context is that he happens to see a book. So, he uses his experience in answering the question. On the other hand, adults often say, I don’t know. They seem to be slaves of a language. In this sense, Participant A did a very good job.
Participant B had to interpret 3:15. What he knew as the context was that the conversation had happened in the clinic. He had to use the context to interpret 3:15. Unfortunately, he failed to try. Though the teacher gave him some cues, he was not successful. In other words, he was not context-sensitive.
Participant C selected Put it back before someone notices. She was good at translating the first part, Put it back. Because the L2 usage and the L1 usage were very similar, she quickly responded to the L1 translation. The problem was the second part: before someone notices. She translated notices with its linguistic meaning. It sounded awkward. She could have used her imagination in such a situation. Still, she could not think of the appropriate Korean usage and she gave up.
In sum, Participant A was excellent at using context. He did very well in extending the linguistic meaning into the pragmatic meaning. Participant B, unlike Participant A, was logical in his thinking, but he could not picture a virtual world. Participant C was in between Participants A and B in this regard.
This activity showed that films, including movies and TV drama series, cannot be interpreted without appropriate context. Social interaction is not exceptional. Therefore, teachers must instruct and train their students better in using films for L2 learning.

3. Third Activity: Recognition of the Importance of World Knowledge

Most EFL students tend to think that they must obviously focus on language matters in their L2 learning. When a film is used in the EFL classroom, some students often complain about a lack of language focus. As mentioned earlier, people use language to talk about the real world, not a language.
In the third activity, we investigated how much non-linguistic matters the participants were involved in. When they handed in their reports, the teacher asked them contextual questions based on the information implied in their selected conventional expressions. The results are shown in Table 4.
Participant A selected the conventional expression You’re the last person I would have expected to be here. He explained the last person with the following sample sentence: This is the last thing your father would have wanted. He focused on a language matter, the last person and would have + p.p. This was natural. The teacher asked him, “When you do something wrong in school, who will take care of your punishment?” The teacher and Participant A had a talk about this. After the conversation, Participant A confessed that their conversation gave salience to the form “would have expected.” He thought that such conversation was not relevant to language learning in the first place, but he changed his mind because of its salience.
The teacher asked Participant B the same question that he asked Participant A. He continued to ask him whether “Anything that happens here” has a positive meaning or a negative meaning. Also, he asked again, “If it is negatively expressed, what would the reason be?” Participant B made one comment: he said he was surprised that with nonlinguistic knowledge, it was possible to learn linguistic knowledge. Participant C selected I got George in my office. The teacher asked her two questions: first, why “my office” was used instead of the hospital; and second, why “got” was used. She did not answer the first question, but she answered the second question correctly. Because office and hospital are both places, got was easily inferred.
In sum, world knowledge or contextual knowledge intervened and helped the participants to understand the meanings of other lexical items. Also, such knowledge gave salience to conventional expressions, and consequently, the participants were helped to improve their language learning. In the first activity, the participants performed poorly. Had they handled the first activity well, they could have learned grammatical knowledge. Without separating lexicon and grammar, they would have accessed grammar. In the second activity, they performed relatively well. In the third activity, they needed their teacher’s assistance.

V. CONCLUSION AND PEDAGOGICAL IMPLICATION

According to the usage-based approach, actual instances from the real world are keys to mastering a language (Langacker, 2008). Films, including TV series, take place in a virtual world, which is not real. In this sense, using films to learn a second language is relevant to success.
In this paper, it was observed how the participants used TV drama series (here, Good Witch). They primarily focused on understanding the meaning of the script. They could not be blamed for this because L2 learners could not handle English utterances without L1 translation. The problem was that the participants stopped learning after the translation. They thought they had completed their L2 learning. In this way, they could not use utterances from TV series in real conversations because they happened to learn the English receptively. Such knowledge needed to be turned into productive knowledge (Faraj, 2015; Webb, 2009). In addition, the transmedia approach was to show and present students the film and help them to understand how to approach different media platforms during the study. The reason the transmedia approach was not specifically mentioned in the study is because it is part of the process not to consciously alert the participants about the procedure.
Another issue was their translation. The participants often translated linguistically, and they were satisfied. Willits et al. (2015) argued that human beings use language to talk about the world, such as about entities and events. Thus, the participants should have expressed entities and events in the world with world knowledge. In other words, they had to do a contextual translation. As is known, film is a context-sensitive medium (Mehmet, 2019; Wang, 2012; Webb, 2010). For correct form-meaning pairing, contextual or pragmatic knowledge should be used (Willits et al., 2015). The third issue was the participants’ choices of utterances from the script. They tended to pay too much attention to idiosyncratic or idiomatic utterances instead of picking up schematic expressions. Technically speaking, they preferred highly specific expressions to highly schematic ones. Consequently, they lost grammatical insight.
Of the three activities that the participants performed, they were poor in the first activity. This activity was supposed to improve their grammatical power, even though they did not use school grammar. In fact, they were slightly clumsy in talking about metalinguistic knowledge. This might be because they were not trained to develop grammatical insight. They preferred specific expressions, such as idiomatic and idiosyncratic expressions, too much. This caused them to seriously lose grammatical insight. In other words, they were accustomed to focusing on the meanings of utterances, not on their patterns.
In the second activity, they performed better at pragmatic translation in that the pragmatic knowledge was similar in both the L1 and L2 learning. However, they needed their teacher’s guidance because it was not easy for them to perform L1 pragmatic translation alone.
The pedagogical implication of this insight to EFL teachers who use films, including TV series, is that in teaching film-based materials, the goal should be both to enjoy watching them and to learn conventional expressions. The ideal length of time for watching a film is between 3 min and 5 min. If the watching time lasts longer than this, students tend to relax and simply watch the film.
In fact, students prefer focusing on language matters even if the teacher uses films. A teacher should keep in mind what to teach. The teaching target is no doubt conventional expressions. However, it should be more than that. Through conventional expressions, students must learn grammar, culture, and pragmatics. If they learn only conventional expressions, they might complain. Students expect much-both contextual learning and cultural learning-focused on language matters when using film materials.
Finally, students like to learn idiomatic and idiosyncratic expressions. However, such expressions are less productive. Schematic expressions that are productive should be cared for. However, this does not mean that idiomatic and idiosyncratic expressions should not be taught but that all the expressions, schematic or specific, should be treated equally in terms of construction.

Notes

1. A university in Korea has been offering movie courses for over 20 years.

2. The dialogue is the first part of The Favourite (Lanthimos, 2018). It is cited in Ryu (2022, p.19).

TABLE 1
Degrees of Schematicity and Specificity
Degree of schematicity Degree of specificity Examples
Fully filled and fixed Fully specific Blue moon, by and large, children, ink, blue
Fully filled and partially flexible Partially specific Go [tense] postal, hit [tense] the road
Partially filled Partially specific The [AdjP] (e.g., the rich/hungry/young)
[time expression] ago (e.g., six days/beers ago)
Fully schematic [V NP]VP, [NP VP]S
Stem V-PAST (e.g., walk-ed, smell-ed)
TABLE 2
First Activity on Good Witch
Participant Conventional expression Degree of schematicity Degree of specificity
A Next time your band is in town, take them by the bistro. X One of these days, I’ll take you all to the restaurant.
B It’s no secret that you weren’t my first choice to replace Jake as Chief. X X
C You left out a few things. I left out the carrots because I don’t like them. X
TABLE 3
Second Activity on Good Witch
Participant Conventional expression Linguistic meaning → Pragmatic meaning
A I guess the dedication ceremony brings up stuff. I guess → L1 translating implicitly
brings up → meaning extension
B I’m also your 3:15. 3:15 → adding contextual information
C Put it back before someone notices. notices → meaning extension
TABLE 4
Third Activity on Good Witch
Participant Conventional expression Contextual information (culture)
A You’re the last person I would have expected to be here, Grace. This dialogue is between the vice president of the school and Grace. Have you ever been summoned to the vice president’s office?
B Anything that happens here becomes part of your permanent record. What does “Anything that happens here” mean?
C I got George in my office. Why did Sam say “my office” instead of “the hospital”?

REFERENCES

Ausubel, D. (1963). The psychology of meaningful verbal learning. New York, NY:Grune & Stratton.
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2009). Conventional expressions as a pragmalinguistic resource: Recognition and production of conventional expressions in L2 pragmatics. Language Learning, 59(4), 755-795. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00525.x .
crossref
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2012). Formulars, routines, and conventional expressions in pragmatic research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 32, 206-227. https://doi.org/10.1017/SO267190512000086 .
crossref
Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2014). Awareness of meaning of conventional expressions in second language pragmatics. Language Awareness, 23(1-2), 41-56. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658416.2013.863894 .
crossref
Bardovi-Harlig, K., & Vellenga, H. E. (2012). The effect of instruction on conventional expressions in L2 pragmatics. System, 40, 77-89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2012.01.004 .
crossref
Beare, K. (2019). October 14; Nonreversible word pairs. Retrieved from https://www.thoughtco.com/nonreversible-word-pairs-1209931 .
Bybee, J. L. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge, UK:Cambridge University Press.
Diessel, H. (2015). Usage-based construction grammar. In E. Dabrowska & D. Diviak (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics. pp 296-322. Berlin, Germany:de Gruyter Mouton: https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110292022-015 .
crossref
Donaghy, K. (2014). October 21; How can film help you teach or learn English?. British Council: Retrieved from https://www.britishcouncil.org/voices-magazine/how-can-film-help-you-teach-or-learn-english .
Dynel, M. (2017). “Is there a tumour in your humour?”: On misunderstanding and miscommunication in conversational humour. In R. Gioria & M. Haugh (Eds.), Doing pragmatics interculturally: Cognitive, philosophical, and sociopragmatic perspectives. pp 55-78. Berlin, Germany:de Gruyter Mouton.
crossref
Edmonds, A. (2014). Conventional expressions: Investigating pragmatics and processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36, 69-99. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263113000557 .
crossref
Ellis, N. C. (2013). Construction grammar and second language acquisition. In T. Hoffmann & G. Trousdale (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of construction grammar. Oxford, UK:Oxford University Press: https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396683.013.0020 .
crossref
Ellis, N. C., & Wulff, S. (2015). Usage-based approaches to SLA. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction. pp 75-93. New York, NY:Routledge.
Faraj, A. K. A. (2015). Effective strategies for turning receptive vocabulary into productive vocabulary in EFL context. Journal of Education and Practice, 6(27), 10-20.
Fleming, L. (2013). Expanding learning opportunities with transmedia practices: Inanimate Alice as an exemplar. Journal of Media Literacy Education, 5(2), 370-377. https://doi.org/10.23860/jmle-5-2-3 .
crossref
Fried, M. (2015). Construction grammar. In T. Kiss & A. Alexiadou (Eds.), Syntax-theory and analysis: An international handbook (. 2, pp 974-1003. Berlin, Germany:de Gruyter Mouton.
crossref
Gawron, J.-M. (2019). Frame semantics. In C. Maienborn & K. von Heusinger & P. Portner (Eds.), Semantics theories. pp 57-85. Berlin, Germany:de Gruyter Mouton: https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110589245-003 .
crossref
Goodwin, C., & Duranti, A. (1992). Rethinking context: An introduction. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (Eds.), Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon. pp 1-42. Cambridge, UK:Cambridge University Press.
Hilpert, M. (2020). Construction grammar and its application to English. (2nd ed). Edinburgh, UK:Edinburgh University Press: https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.00070ost .
Ibbotson, P. (2013). The scope of usage-based theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 255 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00255 .
crossref pmid pmc
Jenkins, H. (2006). Convergence culture: Where old and new media collide. New York, NY:New York University Press.
Kádár, D. Z., & House, J. (2021). Conventionalization and conventions. In M. Haugh & D.Z. Kádár & M. Terkourafi (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of sociopragmatics. pp 162-181. Cambridge, UK:Cambridge University Press: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108954105 .
crossref
Kecskes, I. (2021). Sociocognitive pragmatics. In M. Haugh & D.Z. Kádár & M. Terkourafi (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of sociopragmatics. pp 592-615. Cambridge, UK:Cambridge University Press: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108954105 .
crossref
Koyfman, S. (2021). January 21; How to use movies to learn a language. Babbel Magazine. Retrieved from https://www.babbel.com/en/magazine/use-foreign-movies-for-language-learning/ .
Langacker, R. W. (1988). A usage-based model. In B. Rudzka-Ostyn (Ed.), Topics in cognitive linguistics. pp 127-163. Amsterdam, the Netherlands:Benjamins.
crossref
Langacker, R. W. (2008). Cognitive grammar as a basis for language instruction. In P. Robinson & N.C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition. pp 66-88. New York, NY:Routledge.
Lanthimos, Y. Director (2018). The favourite [Motion picture]. United States:Searchlight Pictures..
Lee, Y. (2020). Movie English and exemplar-based learning: Based on the movie Push . STEM Journal, 21(3), 47-72. https://doi.org/10.16875/stem.2020.21.3.47 .
crossref
Lee, Y. (2022). A study of the effects of transmedia storytelling on active participation and language learning. Journal of English Teaching through Movies and Media, 23(2), 29-40. https://doi.org/10.16875/stem.2022.23.2.29 .
crossref
Lemmens, M. (2016). Cognitive semantics. In R. Nick (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of semantics. pp 90-105. London:Routledge.
Lewis, M. (1993). The lexical approach: The state of ELT and the way forward. Hove, UK:Language Teaching..
Locher, M. A., & Graham, S. L. (2021). Interpersonal pragmatics. In M. Haugh & D.Z. Kádár & M. Terkourafi (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of sociopragmatics. pp 569-591. Cambridge, UK:Cambridge University Press: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108954105 .
crossref
Louwerse, M. M. (2008). Embodied relations are encoded in language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 838-844. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.4.838 .
crossref pmid
Lynn, L. Director (2015a). Running scared [Television series episode]. In C. Pryce & S. Tenney (Eds.), Good witch. Toronto:Whizbang Films.
Lynn, L. Director (2015b). Do the right thing [Television series episode]. In C. Pryce & S. Tenney (Eds.), Good witch. Toronto:Whizbang Films.
Mehmet, U. (2019). The approach of learning a foreign language by watching TV series. Educational Research and Reviews, 14(17), 608-617. https://doi.org/10.5897/ERR2019.3839 .
crossref
Pryce, C. Director (2015). Starting over… again [Television series episode]. In C. Pryce & S. Tenney (Eds.), Good witch. Toronto:Whizbang Films.
Realm, M. (2009). September 29; The lexical approach [Blog post]. Retrieved from https://www.myenglishpages.com/blog/lexical-approach/ .
Rosson, N. (2022). May 6; How to use movies to learn a language: 5 great tips! [Blog post]. Retrieved from https://ling-app.com/tips/how-to-use-movies-to-learn-a-language/ .
Ryu, D. (2022). A study on roles of proper names in movie English. Journal of English Teaching through Movies and Media, 23(1), 13-26. https://doi.org/10.16875/stem.2022.23.1.13 .
crossref
Schmid, H.-J. (2020). The dynamics of the linguistic system: Usage, conventionalization, and entrenchment. Oxford, UK:Oxford University Press.
Shore, D. Creator 2004-2012 House M.D. [Television series]. New York, NY:Fox.
Taylor, J. R. (2012). The mental corpus: How language is represented in the mind. Oxford, UK:Oxford University Press.
Vujic, J. L. (2016). Construction grammar and foreign/second language teaching. Journal of Language, Literature, Arts and Culture, 12(33), 9-19. https://doi.org/10.5937/naslkg1633009v .
crossref
Wang, Y.-C. (2012). Learning L2 vocabulary with American TV drama: From the learner’s perspective. English Language Teaching, 5(8), 217-225. https://doi.org/10.5539/elt.v5n8p217 .
crossref
Webb, S. (2009). The effects of receptive and productive learning of word pairs on vocabulary knowledge. Regional Language Centre Journal, 40(3), 360-376. https://doi.org/10,1177/0033688209343854 .
crossref
Webb, S. (2010). Selecting television programs for language learning: Investigating television programs from the same genre. International Journal of English Studies, 11(1), 117-135.
crossref pdf
Willits, J. A., Amato, M. S., & MacDonald, M. C. (2015). Language knowledge and event knowledge in language use. Cognitive Psychology, 78, 1-27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.02.002 .
crossref pmid pmc
Wulff, S., & Nick, C. E. (2018). Usage-based approaches to second language acquisition. In D. Miller & F. Bayram & J. Rothman & L. Serratrice (Eds.), Bilingual cognition and language: The state of the science across its subfields. pp 37-56. Amsterdam, the Netherlands:John Benjamins: https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.54.03wul .
crossref
TOOLS
METRICS Graph View
  • 1 Crossref
  •  0 Scopus
  • 1,316 View
  • 49 Download
Related articles


ABOUT
BROWSE ARTICLES
EDITORIAL POLICY
FOR CONTRIBUTORS
Editorial Office
#1219, Bugak building, Kookmin University,
Jeongneung-ro 77, Seongbuk-gu, Seoul 02707, Korea
E-mail: stem@stemedia.co.kr                

Copyright © 2024 by The Society for Teaching English through Media.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer
prev next